Quantcast
ZME Science
  • News
  • Environment
  • Health
  • Future
  • Space
  • Features
    Menu
    Natural Sciences
    Health
    History & Humanities
    Space & Astronomy
    Technology
    Culture
    Resources
    Natural Sciences

    Physics

    • Matter and Energy
    • Quantum Mechanics
    • Thermodynamics

    Chemistry

    • Periodic Table
    • Applied Chemistry
    • Materials
    • Physical Chemistry

    Biology

    • Anatomy
    • Biochemistry
    • Ecology
    • Genetics
    • Microbiology
    • Plants and Fungi

    Geology and Paleontology

    • Planet Earth
    • Earth Dynamics
    • Rocks and Minerals
    • Volcanoes
    • Dinosaurs
    • Fossils

    Animals

    • Mammals
    • Birds
    • Fish
    • Reptiles
    • Amphibians
    • Invertebrates
    • Pets
    • Conservation
    • Animals Facts

    Climate and Weather

    • Climate Change
    • Weather and Atmosphere

    Geography

    Mathematics

    Health
    • Drugs
    • Diseases and Conditions
    • Human Body
    • Mind and Brain
    • Food and Nutrition
    • Wellness
    History & Humanities
    • Anthropology
    • Archaeology
    • Economics
    • History
    • People
    • Sociology
    Space & Astronomy
    • The Solar System
    • The Sun
    • The Moon
    • Planets
    • Asteroids, Meteors and Comets
    • Astronomy
    • Astrophysics
    • Cosmology
    • Exoplanets and Alien Life
    • Spaceflight and Exploration
    Technology
    • Computer Science & IT
    • Engineering
    • Inventions
    • Sustainability
    • Renewable Energy
    • Green Living
    Culture
    • Culture and Society
    • Bizarre Stories
    • Lifestyle
    • Art and Music
    • Gaming
    • Books
    • Movies and Shows
    Resources
    • How To
    • Science Careers
    • Metascience
    • Fringe Science
    • Science Experiments
    • School and Study
    • Natural Sciences
    • Health
    • History and Humanities
    • Space & Astronomy
    • Culture
    • Technology
    • Resources
  • Reviews
  • More
    • Agriculture
    • Anthropology
    • Biology
    • Chemistry
    • Electronics
    • Geology
    • History
    • Mathematics
    • Nanotechnology
    • Economics
    • Paleontology
    • Physics
    • Psychology
    • Robotics
  • About Us
    • About
    • The Team
    • Advertise
    • Contribute
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact
No Result
View All Result
ZME Science

No Result
View All Result
ZME Science

Home → Science → News

Pain, the self-fulfilling prophecy: When we expect something to hurt, it does

Mind over body.

Tibi Puiu by Tibi Puiu
February 22, 2019
in Neurology, News

pain treshold
Credit: Pixabay.

Our expectations have far-reaching consequences: they can influence learning and sports performance — and, apparently, how we respond to pain. According to researchers at the University of Colorado, Boulder, expecting a certain pain intensity can become a self-fulfilling prophecy. For instance, expecting a vaccine shot to hurt will probably result in this feeling — even if that needle poke isn’t really painful at all. What’s more, this response persists even when reality demonstrates otherwise.

“We discovered that there is a positive feedback loop between expectation and pain,” said senior author Tor Wager, a professor of psychology and neuroscience at the University of Colorado Boulder. “The more pain you expect, the stronger your brain responds to the pain. The stronger your brain responds to the pain, the more you expect.”

Wager and colleagues wanted to investigate why pain expectations are so entrenched and resistant to change. The researchers were inspired to carry out the new study after noticing that test subjects participating in Wager’s lab for various research often continued to expect something to hurt badly even though they were shown time and time again that the stimulus wasn’t painful at all.

The research team recruited 34 volunteers who were taught to associate one symbol with low heat and another with high, painful heat. Each participant was then seated inside a fMRI machine that records neural activity. For an hour, the subjects were shown various cues representing low or high pain — the symbols that they were trained to associate earlier, but also words like “Low” and “High”, or the letter L and W —  and then rated how much pain they expected to receive.

Next, a painful but non-damaging heat was applied to the volunteers’ forearm or leg, then they were asked to rate the pain. The most painful stimuli was “about what it feels like to hold a hot cup of coffee” according to Wager.

The volunteers weren’t told that there was actually no relationship between the intensity of heat they felt and the preceding cues. Nevertheless, when the subjects expected more pain due to the cues, brain regions associated with threat detection and fear became activated. When they actually received the stimulus, brain regions involved in the generation of pain lit up more. Regardless of how much heat they actually received, the participants generally reported more pain following exposure to high-pain cues.

“This suggests that expectations had a rather deep effect, influencing how the brain processes pain,” said Marieke Jepma, then a postdoctoral researcher in Wager’s lab.

And it seems like the brain has even more tricks to play on us. When the participants expected high pain and got it, the next time they would expect even more pain. However, if they expected high pain and didn’t get it, nothing changed — an example of confirmation bias in action. Confirmation bias can prevent us from considering other information when making decisions since we tend to only see factors that support our beliefs.

“You would assume that if you expected high pain and got very little you would know better the next time. But interestingly, they failed to learn,” said Wager.

The findings could prove important for medicine in settings where pain management is important. For instance, the study may explain why in some instances patients whose damaged tissues have healed still have chronic pain.  

“Our results suggest that negative expectations about pain or treatment outcomes may in some situations interfere with optimal recovery, both by enhancing perceived pain and by preventing people from noticing that they are getting better,” Jepma said. “Positive expectations, on the other hand, could have the opposite effects.”

The findings appeared in the journal Nature Human Behaviour.

Was this helpful?


Thanks for your feedback!

Related posts:
  1. A self fulfilling prophecy – young men’s self esteem hurt by female companion success
  2. EPA ends important Clean Air policy, fulfilling request of fossil fuel lobby
  3. Posting edited photos on social media could hurt your self-esteem
  4. What does Biden have in store for science? Expect changes on COVID-19 and climate change, for starters
  5. Experiment shows that crabs and lobsters feel pain, suggests we don’t really understand animal pain
Tags: pain

ADVERTISEMENT
  • News
  • Environment
  • Health
  • Future
  • Space
  • Features
  • Reviews
  • More
  • About Us

© 2007-2021 ZME Science - Not exactly rocket science. All Rights Reserved.

No Result
View All Result
  • News
  • Environment
  • Health
  • Future
  • Space
  • Features
    • Natural Sciences
    • Health
    • History and Humanities
    • Space & Astronomy
    • Culture
    • Technology
    • Resources
  • Reviews
  • More
    • Agriculture
    • Anthropology
    • Biology
    • Chemistry
    • Electronics
    • Geology
    • History
    • Mathematics
    • Nanotechnology
    • Economics
    • Paleontology
    • Physics
    • Psychology
    • Robotics
  • About Us
    • About
    • The Team
    • Advertise
    • Contribute
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact

© 2007-2021 ZME Science - Not exactly rocket science. All Rights Reserved.

Don’t you want to get smarter every day?

YES, sign me up!

Over 35,000 subscribers can’t be wrong. Don’t worry, we never spam. By signing up you agree to our privacy policy.

✕
ZME Science News

FREE
VIEW