ZME Science
No Result
View All Result
ZME Science
No Result
View All Result
ZME Science

Home → Environment

Jellyfish can learn just like humans — even though they lack a brain

Turns out, jellyfish are not just simple, pulsating blobs—they can learn from experience.

Fermin KoopbyFermin Koop
October 2, 2023
in Animals, Biology, Environment, News
A A
Edited and reviewed by Zoe Gordon
Share on FacebookShare on TwitterSubmit to Reddit

Jellyfish are much more advanced than we thought. A new study has shown that the Caribbean box jellyfish (Tripedalia cystophora) can learn at a much more complex level, using knowledge from past experiences just like humans, mice and flies. The findings, the researchers said, could change what we know about how cognition works.

jellyfish bell
Image credits: Jan Bielecki.

While they’ve been around for over 500 million years, jellyfish are usually described as simple creatures with limited learning skills. They and their relatives, collectively known as cnidarians, are considered to be the earliest extant animals to develop nervous systems. Jellyfish have no brains, though.

Neurobiologist Anders Garm has been researching box jellyfish for over a decade, a group of jellyfish known for being among the world’s most venomous creatures. But these jellies are also interesting for another reason. It turns out they aren’t as daft as once believed, changing our understanding of what simple nervous systems can do.

“It was once presumed that jellyfish can only manage the simplest forms of learning, including habituation (the ability to get used to a certain stimulation). Now, we see that jellyfish have a much more refined ability to learn, and that they can actually learn from their mistakes,” Garm, one of the study’s authors, said in a news release.

Responding to experience

One feature of more advanced cognitive abilities is changing one’s behavior as a result of experience. In other words, to remember and learn. Garm and his team wanted to test this in box jellyfish. They focused on the Caribbean box jellyfish, a fingernail-sized medusa that lives in mangrove swamps in the Caribbean.

First, the researchers decorated a round tank with grey and white stripes to simulate the jellyfish’s natural habitat, with grey stripes mimicking mangrove roots. At first, jellyfish swam close to the stripes and bumped into them. However, by the end of the experiment (7.5 minutes later), the jellyfish increased their average distance from the wall by about 50%.

Image credits: Bieleki et al.

Jellyfish quadrupled the number of successful pivots to avoid collision and reduced their contact with the wall by half, suggesting they can learn from experience through visual stimuli.

“We can see that as each new day of hunting begins, box jellyfish learn from the current contrasts by combining visual impressions and sensations,” Garm said.

RelatedPosts

Comb jellies could be the earliest ancestors of all animals
Why a jellyfish is the ocean’s most efficient swimmer [with video]
Scientists identify the oldest known species of swimming jellyfish
Moon Jellyfish morphs back into symmetry after losing limbs

The researchers then wanted to identify the underlying process of the jellyfish’s associative learning by isolating the animal’s visual sensory centers, known as rhopalia. These sensory structures carry six eyes and trigger pacemaker signals that coordinate the jelly’s pulsing motion, which increases in frequency when the animal veers away from obstacles.

They isolated individual rhopalia and placed them in front of moving grey bars to mimic approaching objects. At first, the structure didn’t respond to light grey bars, interpreting them as distant. However when stimulated with a combination of visual cues and weak electric signals, these structures generated responses that helped the jellyfish avoid obstacles, proving that even with a very basic nervous system, these animals can learn through a form of ‘associative learning.’

“Despite having a mere one thousand nerve cells, they can connect temporal convergences of various impressions and learn a connection — or what we call associative learning,” Garm said.

The next steps

The study challenges previous scientific perceptions of what animals with simple nervous systems can do. For overall neuroscience, it’s really big news, Garm said. “This suggests that advanced learning may have been one of the most important evolutionary benefits of the nervous system from the very beginning,” he added.

Future research will delve into how exactly these jellyfish store memories and further explore their sensory systems.

“It’s surprising how fast these animals learn; it’s about the same pace as advanced animals are doing,” Garm said in a statement.

The study was published in the journal Current Biology.

Tags: jellyfish

ShareTweetShare
Fermin Koop

Fermin Koop

Fermin Koop is a reporter from Buenos Aires, Argentina. He holds an MSc from Reading University (UK) on Environment and Development and is specialized in environment and climate change news.

Related Posts

Environmental Issues

A swarm of jellyfish just shut down 10% of France’s nuclear power

byMihai Andrei
5 days ago
Invertebrates

7 Extraordinary Jellyfish That Prove You Don’t Need a Spine to Be Awesome

byShiella Olimpos
3 months ago
Animals

This Benjamin Button-like Jellyfish Can Age in Reverse, From Adult to Juvenile

byTibi Puiu
12 months ago
Artistic reconstruction of a group of Burgessomedusa phasmiformis swimming in the Cambrian sea. Image credits: Christian McCall.
Animals

Scientists identify the oldest known species of swimming jellyfish

byFermin Koop
2 years ago

Recent news

The UK Government Says You Should Delete Emails to Save Water. That’s Dumb — and Hypocritical

August 16, 2025

In Denmark, a Vaccine Is Eliminating a Type of Cervical Cancer

August 16, 2025
This Picture of the Week shows a stunning spiral galaxy known as NGC 4945. This little corner of space, near the constellation of Centaurus and over 12 million light-years away, may seem peaceful at first — but NGC 4945 is locked in a violent struggle. At the very centre of nearly every galaxy is a supermassive black hole. Some, like the one at the centre of our own Milky Way, aren’t particularly hungry. But NGC 4945’s supermassive black hole is ravenous, consuming huge amounts of matter — and the MUSE instrument at ESO’s Very Large Telescope (VLT) has caught it playing with its food. This messy eater, contrary to a black hole’s typical all-consuming reputation, is blowing out powerful winds of material. This cone-shaped wind is shown in red in the inset, overlaid on a wider image captured with the MPG/ESO telescope at La Silla. In fact, this wind is moving so fast that it will end up escaping the galaxy altogether, lost to the void of intergalactic space. This is part of a new study that measured how winds move in several nearby galaxies. The MUSE observations show that these incredibly fast winds demonstrate a strange behaviour: they actually speed up far away from the central black hole, accelerating even more on their journey to the galactic outskirts. This process ejects potential star-forming material from a galaxy, suggesting that black holes control the fates of their host galaxies by dampening the stellar birth rate. It also shows that the more powerful black holes impede their own growth by removing the gas and dust they feed on, driving the whole system closer towards a sort of galactic equilibrium. Now, with these new results, we are one step closer to understanding the acceleration mechanism of the winds responsible for shaping the evolution of galaxies, and the history of the universe. Links  Research paper in Nature Astronomy by Marconcini et al. Close-up view of NGC 4945’s nucleus

Astronomers Find ‘Punctum,’ a Bizarre Space Object That Might be Unlike Anything in the Universe

August 15, 2025
  • About
  • Advertise
  • Editorial Policy
  • Privacy Policy and Terms of Use
  • How we review products
  • Contact

© 2007-2025 ZME Science - Not exactly rocket science. All Rights Reserved.

No Result
View All Result
  • Science News
  • Environment
  • Health
  • Space
  • Future
  • Features
    • Natural Sciences
    • Physics
      • Matter and Energy
      • Quantum Mechanics
      • Thermodynamics
    • Chemistry
      • Periodic Table
      • Applied Chemistry
      • Materials
      • Physical Chemistry
    • Biology
      • Anatomy
      • Biochemistry
      • Ecology
      • Genetics
      • Microbiology
      • Plants and Fungi
    • Geology and Paleontology
      • Planet Earth
      • Earth Dynamics
      • Rocks and Minerals
      • Volcanoes
      • Dinosaurs
      • Fossils
    • Animals
      • Mammals
      • Birds
      • Fish
      • Amphibians
      • Reptiles
      • Invertebrates
      • Pets
      • Conservation
      • Animal facts
    • Climate and Weather
      • Climate change
      • Weather and atmosphere
    • Health
      • Drugs
      • Diseases and Conditions
      • Human Body
      • Mind and Brain
      • Food and Nutrition
      • Wellness
    • History and Humanities
      • Anthropology
      • Archaeology
      • History
      • Economics
      • People
      • Sociology
    • Space & Astronomy
      • The Solar System
      • Sun
      • The Moon
      • Planets
      • Asteroids, meteors & comets
      • Astronomy
      • Astrophysics
      • Cosmology
      • Exoplanets & Alien Life
      • Spaceflight and Exploration
    • Technology
      • Computer Science & IT
      • Engineering
      • Inventions
      • Sustainability
      • Renewable Energy
      • Green Living
    • Culture
    • Resources
  • Videos
  • Reviews
  • About Us
    • About
    • The Team
    • Advertise
    • Contribute
    • Editorial policy
    • Privacy Policy
    • Contact

© 2007-2025 ZME Science - Not exactly rocket science. All Rights Reserved.