homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Despite their amazing achievements, leaf-cutter ants don't coordinate work

Work hard, not smart!

Alexandru Micu
January 24, 2019 @ 3:43 pm

share Share

There’s no work quite like teamwork — unless you’re a leaf-cutter ant.


Photo by Scott Bauer, public domain.

“Leaf-cutter ant” is a catch-many term referring to some 47 species of ants in 2 genera, Atta and Acromyrex, found in Central and South America. They all share the same way of life, feeding on fungi/mushrooms that they themselves grow in their mounds.

But agriculture is hard work, and involves proper fertilization of crops to reap the full rewards. The ants know this, and much of their day is spent on cutting leaves (uncanny, I know), flowers, and other types of plant matter to use as a substrate for their ‘fields’. Another big part of the mound’s operations, therefore, involves the construction and proper maintenance of infrastructure — ‘superhighways’ hundreds of meters long — that allow easy access to and from the raw resources these ants require.

Leaf-cutter ants form some of the largest and most complex communities on Earth, perhaps second only to our own. Factor in the scale of their infrastructure and how seamlessly it’s built, and we’ve just assumed that there must be some level of organization among the ants. New research shows, however, that this isn’t the case.

Public works

“Although many thousand individuals contribute to the construction of infrastructure, there is no communication or organisation between them -— the massive foraging trails are a byproduct of the foraging behaviour,” said lead-author Thomas Bochynek from Northwestern University’s department of electrical engineering and computer science.

Leaf-cutter colonies can build nearly three kilometers of trails per year, and invest an average of 11,000 work-hours into constructing and maintaining them. Common wisdom held that such projects are handled through stigmergy — delegating tasks to specialists via direct or indirect communication among individuals. They were believed to leave behind pheromone trails to tell their fellow colony members where extra help is needed in removing a particularly heavy twig, for example.

Bochynek’s team, however, show that the ants handle their infrastructure projects without any coordination whatsoever. Each ant seems to act individually, they write, handling tasks as they are encountered.

Errors may happen, though.
Image credits Geoff Gallice / Wikimedia.

The team monitored colonies of leaf-cutter ants in the wild and in their lab to see how fast they could carve a path through debris and how many individuals would be assigned to the task. They also used computer models to establish the randomized rate of clearance based on each ant’s likelihood of encountering an obstruction by chance and then clearing it.

In essence, this last step gave the team a baseline value for how fast a colony could deal with new tasks. If the ants spent time communicating and organizing to clear a particular spot, the rate would temporarily go down. Once they were all set and got to work, this rate would go up.

Experimental obstruction arena (100 × 50 cm) the team set up in their lab. The trail was blocked with folded blue paper strips (dotted lines, seen magnified in the bottom left). The obstruction arena was placed between the nest (to the left) and the foraging container (to the right).
Image credits Thomas Bochynek et al., (2019), PRSB.

What the team found, however, was an almost linear increase in the number of obstacles removed over time. This didn’t fit the hypothesis that the ants were coordinating. Instead, it suggested that ants went out and handled problems as they encountered them, taking no downtime to communicate and organize. The team believes ants evolved into this particular work ethic because it helps reduce overall energy expenditure for the colony (which means it can keep going on less food) and doesn’t require particular mental oomph from the ants.

“If you do not need to communicate, don’t!”, says Bochynek. “It saves the energetic expenditure of performing the behaviour and reduces the required complexity in the individual animals.”

Needless to say, this is distinctively un-human behavior — but that’s what makes it interesting. Bochynek hopes his team’s results will help spur further research into how insects maximize energy efficiency and handle large-scale projects without oversight.

“Biologists and computer scientists are impressed by the fact that complexity in a group can come from simple individuals because it is so different from our own society,” he said.

“Showing that we can further reduce the minimum amount of complexity required in an individual to still see complex group behaviour is hence exciting, and makes the trail clearing example more impressive in our opinion.”

One area where this research might be particularly interesting is robotics — particularly, robotic swarms and robot cooperation. Bochynek’s study shows that individual complexity is not required for a group to exhibit complex behavior. In other words, that even pretty ‘dumb’ robots working together can perform complex tasks with minimal oversight. Now we just need to write the code.

The paper “Infrastructure construction without information exchange: the trail clearing mechanism in Atta leafcutter ants” has been published in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

share Share

Scientists Say Junk Food Might Be as Addictive as Drugs

This is especially hurtful for kids.

A New AI Can Spot You by How Your Body Bends a Wi-Fi Signal

You don’t need a phone or camera to be tracked anymore: just wi-fi.

Golden Oyster Mushroom Are Invasive in the US. They're Now Wreaking Havoc in Forests

Golden oyster mushrooms, with their sunny yellow caps and nutty flavor, have become wildly popular for being healthy, delicious and easy to grow at home from mushroom kits. But this food craze has also unleashed an invasive species into the wild, and new research shows it’s pushing out native fungi. In a study we believe […]

The World’s Most "Useless" Inventions (That Are Actually Pretty Useful)

Every year, the Ig Nobel Prize is awarded to ten lucky winners. To qualify, you need to publish research in a peer-reviewed journal that is considered "improbable": studies that make people laugh and think at the same time.

This Ancient Greek City Was Swallowed by the Sea—and Yet Refused to Die

A 3,000-year record of resilience, adaptation, and seismic survival

Low testosterone isn't killing your libido. Sugar is

Small increases in blood sugar can affect sperm and sex, even without diabetes

NASA’s Parker Solar Probe Just Flew Closer to the Sun Than Ever Before and the Footage is Breathtaking

Closest-ever solar images offer new insights into Earth-threatening space weather.

The Oldest Dog Breed's DNA Reveals How Humans Conquered the Arctic — and You’ve Probably Never Heard of It

Qimmeq dogs have pulled Inuit sleds for 1,000 years — now, they need help to survive.

A Common DNA Sugar Just Matched Minoxidil in Hair Regrowth Tests on Mice

Is the future of hair regrowth hidden in 2-deoxy-D-ribose?

Your Personal Air Defense System Is Here and It’s Built to Vaporize Up to 30 Mosquitoes per Second with Lasers

LiDAR-guided Photon Matrix claims to fell 30 mosquitoes a second, but questions remain.