homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Why Can't We Remember Our Lives as Babies? Our Earliest Memories May Still be There

New research suggests infants can form memories far earlier than previously thought, but where do they go?

Tibi Puiu
March 21, 2025 @ 8:32 pm

share Share

A baby on a towel

Have you ever wondered why you can’t remember being a baby? Your first words? What about your first steps? Don’t worry, you’re not alone. Psychologists have a special term for the fact that most adults can’t remember much about their early lives, typically up to age 3-4. They call this phenomenon childhood amnesia.

Why do we lose these early memories? Is it because babies can’t form them in the first place, or is something else at play? Turns out that baby humans aren’t the equivalent of a cute blob of goldfish.

Using brain scans of awake infants, researchers have found that the hippocampus — a seahorse-shaped region of the brain that is involved in memory — begins to encode individual experiences as early as one year of age. This challenges a long-held belief that infantile amnesia is due to an immature hippocampus incapable of forming memories. Instead, it suggests that the memories are there, but they become inaccessible as we grow older.

“One really cool possibility is that the memories are actually still there in adulthood. It’s just that we’re not able to access them,” study co-author Tristan Yates, a neuroscientist at Columbia University in New York City, told Nature.

The Mystery of Infantile Amnesia

Even though babies are constantly learning, their memories of specific events seem to vanish. For years, scientists thought this was because the hippocampus, which plays a key role in forming and retrieving memories, wasn’t mature enough to do its job in infancy.

But evidence from animal studies hinted that this might not be the whole story. In rodents, for example, the hippocampus can form memories during infancy, but those memories become inaccessible without direct stimulation. Could the same be true for humans?

To find out, a team of researchers led by Yates, who during this study was a graduate student at Yale University, turned to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). They scanned the brains of 26 infants, ranging from 4 to 24 months old, while the babies viewed a series of photographs. The researchers then tested the infants’ memory by showing them pairs of images — one they had seen before and one that was new — and tracked where the babies looked. If babies stared longer at an image they had seen before, it signaled recognition.

In infants older than 12 months, the hippocampus showed significantly more activity when they viewed images they recognized. This “subsequent memory effect” was particularly strong in the posterior hippocampus, a subregion that plays a role in encoding detailed, episodic memories in adults.

“The hallmark of these types of memories, which we call episodic memories, is that you can describe them to others, but that’s off the table when you’re dealing with pre-verbal infants,” said Nick Turk-Browne, a professor of psychology at Yale and senior author of the study.

Screens showing the scans of the baby brains during the study
Babies had their brains scanned while they performed a memory task.

Statistical Learning

Previously, Turk-Browne found that infants as young as three months old can engage in “statistical learning,” a type of memory that involves extracting patterns from repeated experiences. For example, a baby might learn the typical sequence of events during mealtime or recognize common features of faces. It’s the same mechanism that scientists think is behind some dogs’ ability to memorize and respond to words and phrases. This form of memory relies on the anterior hippocampus, which develops earlier than the posterior region responsible for episodic memory.

“Statistical learning is about extracting the structure in the world around us,” Turk-Browne explained. “This is critical for the development of language, vision, concepts, and more. So, it’s understandable why statistical learning may come into play earlier than episodic memory.”

The new findings suggest that episodic memory emerges later, around the one-year mark, as the posterior hippocampus matures. But if infants can form these memories, why don’t we remember them as adults?

One explanation is that early memories simply fade over time, never making it into long-term storage. Another explanation says that these memories actually persist somewhere in our psyche but become inaccessible. It’s like having a book in a library but losing the card that tells you where to find it. Turk-Browne leans toward the latter, citing recent animal studies.

Can We Get Them Back?

In mice, for example, researchers have shown that early memories can be retrieved by reactivating specific hippocampal neurons, even after they seem to have been forgotten.

“Although mice who learn the location of an escape hole in a water maze during infancy forget this location by maturity, optogenetic reactivation of the hippocampal neurons that were activated during encoding can elicit the learned behavior,” the researchers said.

This is what they’re studying now.  In ongoing experiments, they are testing whether children can recall home videos taken from their perspective as babies. Preliminary results suggest these memories might persist until preschool age before fading — a finding that could reshape our understanding of memory development.

This could also have practical implications. If we can better understand how and why early memories are lost, we might be able to develop interventions to help children with memory impairments or those who have experienced trauma.

So, while the memories of our first years may remain elusive, they are far from forgotten — just hidden, perhaps, in the intricate folds of the developing brain.

The findings appeared in the journal Science.


share Share

Hidden for over a century, a preserved Tasmanian Tiger head "found in a bucket" may bring the lost species back from extinction

Researchers recover vital RNA from Tasmanian tiger, pushing de-extinction closer to reality.

Island Nation Tuvalu Set to Become the First Country Lost to Climate Change. More Than 80% of the Population Apply to Relocate to Australia Under World's First 'Climate Visa'

Tuvalu will likely become the first nation to vanish because of climate change.

Archaeologists Discover 6,000 Year Old "Victory Pits" That Featured Mass Graves, Severed Limbs, and Torture

Ancient times weren't peaceful by any means.

Space Solar Panels Could Cut Europe’s Reliance on Land-Based Renewables by 80 Percent

A new study shows space solar panels could slash Europe’s energy costs by 2050.

A 5,000-Year-Old Cow Tooth Just Changed What We Know About Stonehenge

An ancient tooth reshapes what we know about the monument’s beginnings.

Astronomers See Inside The Core of a Dying Star For the First Time, Confirm How Heavy Atoms Are Made

An ‘extremely stripped supernova’ confirms the existence of a key feature of physicists’ models of how stars produce the elements that make up the Universe.

Rejoice! Walmart's Radioactive Shrimp Are Only a Little Radioactive

You could have a little radioactive shrimp as a treat. (Don't eat any more!)

Newly Found Stick Bug is Heavier Than Any Insect Ever Recorded in Australia

Bigger than a cockroach and lighter than a golf ball, a giant twig emerges from the misty mountains.

Chevy’s New Electric Truck Just Went 1,059 Miles on a Single Charge and Shattered the EV Range Record

No battery swaps, no software tweaks—yet the Silverado EV more than doubled its 493-mile range. How’s this possible?

Dolphins and Whales Can Be Friends and Sometimes Hang Out Together

They have a club and you're not invited.