homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Mongooses remember when their friends helped them -- and help them back

These cute little predators are very social creatures.

Mihai Andrei
May 28, 2018 @ 10:02 pm

share Share

It’s another one of those things that was once thought to be exclusively human but has since been shown to be present in the animal world: dwarf mongooses remember previous cooperative acts by their groupmates and reward them later, according to new work by University of Bristol researchers

Image via Wikipedia.

Don’t let their cute fluffy appearance deceive you — the dwarf mongoose (Helogale parvula) is a predator. It’s Africa’s smallest predator, but still. They live in very social groups of up to 30 individuals, with a strict hierarchy among same-sexed animals within a group, headed by the dominant pair (normally the oldest group members). All group members cooperate in helping to rear the pups and in guarding the group against predators.

Dwarf mongooses also maintain an interesting mutually advantageous relationship with hornbills, in which hornbills seek out the mongooses in order for the two species to forage together more successfully, and to warn each other of nearby predators. So it shouldn’t surprise anyone, really, that the dwarf mongoose is a great communicator and a very social creature. But what did surprise biologists is that not only do these creatures remember when others help them — but they also reward their helper, even after a lot of time has passed. Senior author, Professor Andy Radford from Bristol’s School of Biological Sciences, said:

“Humans frequently trade goods and can track the amount they owe using memories of past exchanges. While nonhuman animals are also known to be capable of trading cooperative acts immediately for one another, more contentious is the possibility that there can be delayed rewards.”

Dwarf mongoose acting as a sentinel (raised guard) a cooperative act that is later rewarded by grooming from groupmates. Image credits: Shannon Wild.

Until now, this type of behavior has only in primate populations, and studies have generally focused on captive populations. Lead author, Dr. Julie Kern, also from Bristol, added:

“There have been hardly any suitable experimental tests on wild animals, especially non-primates. By working with groups of dwarf mongooses habituated to our close presence, we could collect detailed observations and conduct experimental manipulations in natural conditions.”

The study also revealed one of the first cross-commodity trading, whereby individuals reward one type of good or service with another. In this case, grooming was traded for sentinel behavior (depicted above), which involves adopting a high position and keeping a lookout for danger, warning groupmates with alarm calls if something’s wrong.

[Also Read: For banded mongooses, ‘cultural inheritance’ decides what’s for dinner]

Researchers had witnessed this type of behavior several times, but proving a cause-effect relationship was quite tricky, so researchers set up a creative experiment. Professor Radford explains:

“Over three-hour periods when groups were foraging, we simulated extra sentinel behaviour by a subordinate group member using playbacks of its surveillance calls–vocalisations given to announce it is performing this duty. At the sleeping burrow that evening, we monitored all grooming events, especially those received by the individual who had had their sentinel contribution upregulated.”

They found that on days when an individual was perceived to conduct more sentinel duty, it received more evening grooming from groupmates than on regular days (when its foraging calls had been played back during the preceding foraging session). Furthermore, they found that the grooming needn’t occur immediately — sometimes, it took a while before the reward came.

It might seem like the two services don’t quite match up, as being a sentinel seems much harder and more important than being a groomer, but grooming has long been considered an important tradable commodity in social species, being used as a reward or as a sign of affection in various contexts.

Journal Reference: ‘Experimental evidence for delayed contingent cooperation among wild dwarf mongooses’ by J.M. Kern and A.N. Radford in Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA.

share Share

People Living Near Golf Courses Face Double the Risk of Parkinson’s

The strong pesticides sprayed on golf courses leech into the groundwater and scientists suspect this could increase the risk of Parkinson's.

He Let Snakes Bite Him Over 200 Times and Now Scientists Want His Blood for an Universal Antivenom

A universal snakebite treatment may be within reach, thanks to an unlikely human experiment.

These companies want to make hand bags out of T-rex leather. But scientists aren't buying it

A lab-grown leather inspired by dinosaur skin sparks excitement—and scientific skepticism

This car-sized "millipede" was built like a tank — and had the face to go with it

A Carboniferous beast is showing its face.

9 Environmental Stories That Don't Get as Much Coverage as They Should

From whales to soil microbes, our planet’s living systems are fraying in silence.

Scientists Find CBD in a Common Brazilian Shrub That's Not Cannabis

This wild plant grows across South America and contains CBD.

Spruce Trees Are Like Real-Life Ents That Anticipate Solar Eclipse Hours in Advance and Sync Up

Trees sync their bioelectric signals like they're talking to each other.

The Haast's Eagle: The Largest Known Eagle Hunted Prey Fifteen Times Its Size

The extinct bird was so powerful it could kill a 400-pound animal with its talons.

Miracle surgery: Doctors remove a hard-to-reach spinal tumor through the eye of a patient

For the first time, a deadly spinal tumor has been removed via the eye socket route.

A Lawyer Put a Cartoon Dragon Watermark on Every Page of a Court Filing and The Judge Was Not Amused

A Michigan judge rebukes lawyer for filing documents with cartoon dragon watermark