homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Six-month olds like people who protect the weak, suggesting we're born with a love for superheroes

We may be born with an innate sense of right and wrong.

Alexandru Micu
April 7, 2017 @ 3:19 pm

share Share

Humans may have an innate feel for ‘wrong’ and ‘right,’ new research suggests. The paper describes how infants seem to recognize heroic acts even before reaching verbal ages. This might also help explain the popularity heroes enjoy in cultures throughout the world.

Superbaby.

Image credits Andrew Bishop / Flickr.

There’s just something about heroes that make you go all fuzzy inside, isn’t there? From the Spartans in 300 to Batman, we look up to them and root for them through impossible odds and poorly lit alleyways. If you take a moment to think about it, though, those Spartans are doing the exact thing as the Persians — murdering the other guys. Batman himself is pretty shady too, blowing stuff up, kicking people left and right, and generally filling ER rooms to the brim while racking up a huge repairs bill for the city.

So why then do we root for them? Well, there is one thing that sets them apart from the other side — both Batman and the Spartans are doing what they do to defend the weak. According to researchers from Japan, his distinction is enough to turn our brains into groupies — even before we can speak.

M-m-mo-Batman

A team comprised of researchers from the Kyoto and Tokyo Universities led by Masako Myowa found that infants as young as six months of age — under the age of verbalization — show appreciation for figures who take action to protect others.

The study included 132 infants of various age, but the most interesting findings of the study came from work performed with infants under the age of vocalization. Twenty such infants were shown a series of animations with one sphere-with-eyes chasing and then colliding with a similar sphere. A third actor, “a colored cubic geometric agent with eyes” was shown watching this interaction from a distance. The character was represented in a different color depending on what course of action it would take — one version of the animation has this third party intervene following the bump by placing itself between the spheres, and in the other, he simply leaves without taking any action to defend the victim.

After viewing the animation, the infants were given replicas of the intervening and non-intervening character to chose between. Out of the 20 six-month-old infants, 17 chose the green (interfering) cube over the orange (non-interfering) cube. The researchers controlled for differences in attention length — such as the infants looking more at the green cube than the other — and reported “no significant differences for looking time between animations either for the peripheral [or center] area of interest.”

Experiment.

I’d watch this show.
Image credits Kanakogi et al., (2017), Nature.

So the babies allowed both actors the same level of scrutiny and paid both the same amount of attention, but overwhelmingly chose the interfering cube — suggesting a clear preference for this actor based on his action. Since the researchers were working with babies under the age of vocalization, there was a chance they couldn’t distinguish between an accident and a willing act of aggression. So the team performed the experiment again to see if the infants showed preference to the green cube because he merely stopped an unpleasant event, or because he was taking an active social role in protecting someone.

“It is possible that infants in our study regarded the interaction between the spherical figures in mere physical rather than socially aggressive (animate) terms, and as a result preferred the agent that stopped the negative physical event rather than the aggressive interaction per se,” the team writes.

“[So] we eliminated the perceivable ‘social animacy or agency’ of both the interacting spheres by making them appear as if they (i) had no eyes (rather than having eyes), (ii) were non-self-propelled (rather than self-propelled), and (iii) involved no distortion on contact (rather than showing distortion on contact).”

This second experiment also included 20 (new) 6-month-old infants, and they evenly selected between the two cubes — 10 picks for each of them. Just like with the first experiment, there were “no significant differences for looking times between animations.” The third experiment tested whether the infants were choosing the green cube because “he was social”, as it engaged with the spheres regardless if their interaction was negative or positive — and this wasn’t the case.

In short, it seems that there is an innate sense of justice in humans, which only grows more nuanced and complex as infants grow and understand more about justice. The team’s next goal is to track how this understanding develops over time.

“In this study, six-month-olds didn’t show a preference for intentional help over accidental help, whereas ten-month-olds did,” says Professor Myowa.

The paper “Preverbal infants affirm third-party interventions that protect victims from aggressors” has been published in the journal Nature Human Behavior.

 

share Share

Golden Dome Could Cost A Jaw-Dropping $3.6 Trillion. That's More Than Triple The Entire F-35 Program or 100 Times the Manhattan Project

Can America really afford the Golden Dome?

AI Tool Reveals Signs Of Consciousness In Comatose Patients Days Before Doctors Can Detect It

AI tool tracks minute facial movements to detect consciousness in patients previously thought unresponsive.

Teflon Diets, Zebra Cows, and Pizza-Loving Lizards: The 2025 Ig Nobel Prizes Celebrate Weird Science

Science finds humor and insight in the strangest places — from zebra cows to pizza-eating lizards.

Pet sharks have become cool, but is owning them ethical?

When Laurie was a kid, she had recurrent nightmares that featured her getting eaten by a shark. Decades later, Laurie goes to sleep next to them (or at least in the same house). She’s the proud owner of two epaulette sharks (Hemiscyllium ocellatum) in her 1,135-liter (300-gallon) tank: bottom-dwelling spotted sharks up to 0.6 meters […]

Gold, Jade, and a 16-Ton Coffin: The Lost Prince of China’s Terracotta Army May Be Found

A recently discovered hidden coffin in the terracotta army may finally confirm a 2,000-year-old legend.

1% of People Never Have Sex and Genetics Might Explain Why

A study of more than 400,000 people found 1% had never had sex – which was linked to a range of genetic, environmental and other factors.

Researchers Say Humans Are In the Midst of an Evolutionary Shift Like Never Before

Humans are evolving faster through culture than through biology.

Archaeologists Found A Rare 30,000-Year-Old Toolkit That Once Belonged To A Stone Age Hunter

An ancient pouch of stone tools brings us face-to-face with one Gravettian hunter.

Scientists Crack the Secret Behind Jackson Pollock’s Vivid Blue in His Most Famous Drip Painting

Chemistry reveals the true origins of a color that electrified modern art.

China Now Uses 80% Artificial Sand. Here's Why That's A Bigger Deal Than It Sounds

No need to disturb water bodies for sand. We can manufacture it using rocks or mining waste — China is already doing it.