homehome Home chatchat Notifications


The Domesticated Dog’s Ability to Interpret Human Social Cues is a Result of Millennia of Selective Breeding

As if the domesticated dog’s position as, “man’s (or woman’s) best friend” was not entrenched in the human zeitgeist enough, research from Brian Hare out Harvard’s Anthropology department indicates that not only are dogs far more adept than Chimpanzees- our closest genetic, extant relative- at interpreting human social cues, but that domesticated dogs are superior […]

Jason Whitaker
July 20, 2010 @ 5:32 am

share Share

man-looking-like-bulldog

As if the domesticated dog’s position as, “man’s (or woman’s) best friend” was not entrenched in the human zeitgeist enough, research from Brian Hare out Harvard’s Anthropology department indicates that not only are dogs far more adept than Chimpanzees- our closest genetic, extant relative- at interpreting human social cues, but that domesticated dogs are superior to wolves- their closest genetic, extant relative- in this respect, too.

The explanation reached by Hare is thus, “…dogs’ social-communicative skills with humans were acquired during the process of domestication.;” which occurred, because, “…dogs that were able to use social cues to predict the behavior of humans more flexibly than could their last common wolf ancestor … were at a selective advantage.”

In coming to this conclusion, Hare et al. performed four experiments, each pertaining to three hypotheses concerning the genesis of the domesticated dog’s social cognition. The experiments were centered around the Object Choice Task (OCT), where a subject- be it a dog, puppy, or chimpanzee- is presented with two boxes. Hidden underneath one the boxes is a treat (usually a piece of food); and because the box eliminates the possibility of detecting the food by either sight or smell, the subject relies on its ability to interpret the researchers indication (any combination of a gaze, point, or tapping of the box) to choose correctly.

The first of the three hypotheses, The Canid Generalization Hypothesis, posits that many canids, especially wolves, should perform well at OCT tasks because, “…[wolves] typically live in cooperative hunting social groups, making it likely that they need to exploit the behavior of conspecifics [other wolves] and quarry alike, and this ability may then generalize to humans.” The second, “Human Exposure” hypothesis, places the domesticated dog’s ontogeny forefront, implicating their social cognition as the result of having been exposed to humans for their entire lives. Lastly, the Domestication Hypothesis attributes dogs and humans pre-historic relationship as the vehicle of dogs developed ability to interpret human signals: the ability to interpret correctly human intentions, or at least more so than other individuals, was a positive trait. And that over the course of dog and humans 10 millennia long shared history (or, “paraell phylogeny,” if you’d like to be technical), these traits were exacerbated and honed.

Each of these hypotheses possesses logical corollaries. For example, it follows from Canid Generalization that wolves should to as well, or very nearly so, as dogs in OCTs; according to the Human exposure hypothesis puppies reared by humans should outperform their kennel reared peers in OCTs; and if the Domestication Hypothesis were true, dogs should perform well at OCTs regardless of age, since the ability to distinguish human social cues should be greatly inborn (and outperforming Chimpanzees doesn’t hurt to bolster this hypothesis either). By systematically comparing adult dogs and wolves, human reared puppies and kennel reared puppies, and adult dogs and chimpanzees performances at combinations of OCTs, Hare and colleagues were able to shed two of the hypothesis, which left the domestication hypothesis as the clear winner.

Of course, and this is the author writing, our ancestors probably did not selectively breed their dogs on the basis that they could find a hidden piece of food (which is undetectable by direct smell or sight) based on their gesticulations. The most conspicuous answer is that this specific trait (adeptness at OCT tasks) is a function of a dog’s ability to do work. After all, what good is the herding dog that herds on caprice, indiscriminately of his or her owner’s wishes? Simply, humans and canines have benefited from one another for thousands of years, and this relationship is based (like any good relationship) on clear communication. The fact that dogs have retained this ability (because it’s doubted that many of us use our “labradoodles” to herd) is a testament to the two species intimately intertwined lineage. And in the author’s opinion, so long as man (or woman) want their best friend by their side, this trait will persist for another ten millennia.

share Share

Wild Chimpanzees Use Medicine To Treat Each Other’s Wounds

Chimpanzees don’t just treat their own injuries, but care for others, too.

Meet Mosura fentoni, the Bug-Eyed Cambrian Weirdo with Three Eyes and Gills in Its Tail

Evolution went strong in this one.

3,700 Hours with Wild Chimps Reveal Evolutionary Roots of Attachment

This study is about chimps. But in a way, it's also about us.

How Some Flowers Evolved the Grossest Stench — and Why Flies Love It

Flowers keep making the same mutation time and time again.

This car-sized "millipede" was built like a tank — and had the face to go with it

A Carboniferous beast is showing its face.

Spruce Trees Are Like Real-Life Ents That Anticipate Solar Eclipse Hours in Advance and Sync Up

Trees sync their bioelectric signals like they're talking to each other.

This Bizarre Bacterium Conducts Electricity Like a Wire

Conducting electricity as a lifestyle.

Humans are really bad at healing. But that also helped us survive

It's a quirk tied to our thick skin, sweat glands, and sparse body hair.

The world’s largest wildlife crossing is under construction in LA, and it’s no less than a miracle

But we need more of these massive wildlife crossings.

The "Bone Collector" Caterpillar Disguises Itself With the Bodies of Its Victims and Lives in Spider Webs

This insect doesn't play with its food. It just wears it.