homehome Home chatchat Notifications


Pupil dilation can be used to diagnose aphantasia, the lack of visual imagination

This is one of the first reliable pieces of evidence that people with aphantasia are trying to create mental images, but can't.

Alexandru Micu
April 20, 2022 @ 6:47 pm

share Share

Our eyes can help us identify people with aphantasia, according to new research. Aphantasia is a phenomenon in which people are unable to visualize objects or scenes in their minds.

Image via Pixabay.

Researchers at the University of New South Wales, Sydney, report that the pupils of people with aphantasia do not show a response when they are asked to imagine dark and light objects. This can serve as a reliable diagnosis tool, they add, because the pupils of people without aphantasia show a response — called a pupillary reflex — in this situation.

It’s all in the eyes

“The pupillary reflex is an adaptation that optimizes the amount of light hitting the retina,” says Professor Joel Pearson, senior author on the paper. “And while it was already known that imagined objects can evoke so-called ‘endogenous’ changes in pupil size, we were surprised to see more dramatic changes in those reporting more vivid imagery. This really is the first biological, objective test for imagery vividness.”

The researchers worked with 42 individuals who self-reported as having a visual imagination. These participants were then fitted with glasses that tracked their eye movements and the size of their pupils during a series of lab experiments. Throughout these experiments, the participants were presented with bright or dark shapes against a gray background, with the researchers tracking the response of their pupils to these elements.

Bright shapes (which the team explains was comparable to looking up at a bright sky in light intensity) evoked a pupillary constriction response in these patients, while dark shapes caused their pupils to dilate.

The next step involved testing the participants’ capacity to visualize objects; they were asked to imagine the same light or dark shapes from the first step. They had to do this with their eyes open to allow for their pupils to be tracked, and later on report on how vivid those images were in their mind.

This step revealed that the participants’ pupils dilated and constricted appropriately, based on the brightness of the shapes, even when they were only imagining them. Furthermore, the magnitude of their pupillary response corresponded to the vividness of the images in the participants’ minds.

Finally, the team underwent the same steps with 18 participants with self-reported aphantasia. During exposure to the shapes, these showed the same responses as the first group — pupillary constriction to bright shapes and dilation to dark ones. However, they did not show a differentiated response in their pupils during the second experiment, when they were asked to imagine the shapes.

“One of the problems with many existing methods to measure imagery is that they are subjective, that is to say they rely on people being able to accurately assess their own imagery. Our results show an exciting new objective method to measure visual imagery,” says Prof Pearson, “and the first physiological evidence of aphantasia. With over 1.3 million Australians thought to have aphantasia, and 400 million more internationally, we are now close to an objective physiological test, like a blood test, to see if someone truly has it.”

The aphantasic participants were further asked to visualize four shapes at a time to ensure that they were putting as much effort as they could into trying to visualize imagery. The team reports that they did see a difference in pupil activity in the participants at this step. Greater mental effort does have an effect on pupil dilation, the team explains, and this step served to prove that the participants were actually doing their best during the experiment, and not simply pretending to go through the motions.

“Our pupils are known to get larger when we are doing a more difficult task,” says Lachlan Kay, Ph.D. candidate in the Future Minds Lab, UNSW. “Imagining four objects simultaneously is more difficult than imagining just one. The pupils of those with aphantasia dilated when they imagined four shapes compared to one, but did not change based on whether the shapes were bright or dark. This indicated that the participants with aphantasia were indeed trying to imagine in this experiment, just not in a visual way”.”The aphantasic pupil response to the four objects condition is also a really exciting finding,” adds Prof Pearson, “because for the first time we have strong biological evidence that those with aphantasia are really trying to create a mental image, putting to rest claims that they may simply not be attempting to create a mental image.”

The paper “The pupillary light response as a physiological index of aphantasia, sensory and phenomenological imagery strength” has been published in the journal eLife.

share Share

A Former Intelligence Officer Claimed This Photo Showed a Flying Saucer. Then Reddit Users Found It on Google Earth

A viral image sparks debate—and ridicule—in Washington's push for UFO transparency.

This Flying Squirrel Drone Can Brake in Midair and Outsmart Obstacles

An experimental drone with an unexpected design uses silicone wings and AI to master midair maneuvers.

Oldest Firearm in the US, A 500-Year-Old Cannon Unearthed in Arizona, Reveals Native Victory Over Conquistadores

In Arizona’s desert, a 500-year-old cannon sheds light on conquest, resistance, and survival.

No, RFK Jr, the MMR vaccine doesn’t contain ‘aborted fetus debris’

Jesus Christ.

“How Fat Is Kim Jong Un?” Is Now a Cybersecurity Test

North Korean IT operatives are gaming the global job market. This simple question has them beat.

This New Atomic Clock Is So Precise It Won’t Lose a Second for 140 Million Years

The new clock doesn't just keep time — it defines it.

A Soviet shuttle from the Space Race is about to fall uncontrollably from the sky

A ghost from time past is about to return to Earth. But it won't be smooth.

The world’s largest wildlife crossing is under construction in LA, and it’s no less than a miracle

But we need more of these massive wildlife crossings.

Your gold could come from some of the most violent stars in the universe

That gold in your phone could have originated from a magnetar.

Ronan the Sea Lion Can Keep a Beat Better Than You Can — and She Might Just Change What We Know About Music and the Brain

A rescued sea lion is shaking up what scientists thought they knew about rhythm and the brain